Uncommon Descent Serving The Intelligent Design Community

What Does T. cistoides Have To Do With Darwin’s Finches?

Share
Facebook
Twitter
LinkedIn
Flipboard
Print
Email

Because of a prediction, a very strong prediction, I made on another thread, I’ve had reason to look into just what has been happening to Darwin’s finches way off on the Galapagos Islands.

Here is a paper published last year in Science Magazine by the Grants, experts in Darwin’s finches. I looked at their paper, looked at their data, and have come to the conclusion that what I predicted as the ultimate explanation to changed beak sizes is the more reasonable interpretation of the data they present.

But before we even get to the data, here’s a remark from a National Geographic website review of the article that supports my basic position:

“ Researchers from New Jersey’s Princeton University have observed a species of finch in Ecuador’s Galápagos Islands that evolved to have a smaller beak within a mere two decades.
Surprisingly, most of the shift happened within just one generation, the scientists say.”

The shift happened in ONE year? What kind of population genetics are at play here?

Well, to the data:

The most important information that we get from the article (only 3 pages in pdf) are contained in Table 1 and Figure 2.

The Grants’ paper is concerned with how Geospiza fortis and Geospiza magnarostris compete. Their conclusion is that during the drought years of 2003-2004, when the population numbers of both species fell drastically, that the “competition” from G. magnarostris, due to the numbers of both species being almost the same for the first time since G. magnarostris came over from the mainland, caused “character displacement” (beak size change) of G. fortis to a smaller size.

Table 1 gives the number of observed feedings of the three main seed types (small, medium, large) by each of the species. Figure 2 gives the beak size mean of G. fortis over the last 33 years, beginning in 1973-74.

The Grant’s give four ‘lines of support’ for their conclusion. Their fourth ‘line of support’, I believe, becomes the very reason for re-interpreting their results. The fourth ‘line of support’ is that in the drought year 1977, when G. magnarostris were few in numbers, and hence, not able to compete with G. fortis, the beak size of G. fortis actually increased so as to be able to take advantage of the Tribulus cistoides seeds (the large ones), whereas in 2004, with a similar drought taking place, but, however, with G. magnarostris now able to compete with G. fortis, the beak size of G. fortis decreased.

Let’s first notice all of Figure 2. For most of the 33 years that it records beak size, the beak size hardly fluctuates from its mean; there are only two noticeable/significant exemptions: the two drought periods, when, in BOTH instances, beak size changed almost ‘instantaneously’. The title of the National Geographic review had “instant” in its title.

Now to my prediction: my prediction is that changes in the morphology of species is driven mostly, if not completely, by environmental ‘triggers’. Table 1 now becomes important. Notice the difference between the observed feedings of G. fortis between 1977 and 2004. While both were small, the number of feedings on T. cistoides for G. fortis dropped from one sixth for 1977-1989, to one twelfth for 2004—half as much! But that’s not all of the story. In the paper, the Grants indicate that when examing these lowered ‘feedings’, whereas in normal years an average ‘feeding’ on T. cistoides was 9 to 23 mericarps, in 2004 it was never on more than 2 mericarps. That’s a factor of somewhere between 5 and 12. Taken altogether, then, this means that the amount of T. cistoides consumed by G. fortis fell in 2004 to somewhere between one tenth, and one twenty-fourth, of its normal consumption.

Let’s also notice that while both G. fortis and G. magnarostris were decimated, G. magnarostris did not change its beak size to a smaller one. If we look at ‘feeding’ observations for G. magnarostris we see that while they ate a larger proportion of the medium sized seeds than normal, nonetheless, their main intake continued to be T. cistoides.

The more reasonable interpretation of the Grants data is this: there is some protein(s) found in T. cistoides that cause beak size (and other (6) correlated characteristics, see Table 2) to increase, and that in the absence of these protein(s) beak size will diminish. The most likely method of this change is, I believe, through a changed developmental pattern in the next generation. (One possibility is that RNA is involved here, and that somehow the presence of protein(s) that come(s) from the ‘large’ seeds is able to transmit the fact of its presence, or absence, to the subsequent generation).

This thesis, though controversial perhaps, renders what we see—over a 33 year history—sensible, because: (1) It explains why G. magnarostris, despite being decimated by the drought, still does not change—since it is still principally ‘feeding’ on T. cistoides; (2) It explains why G. fortis changes beak size in ONE generation (“Instant” evolution); and (3) most importantly, it explains why in 1977, at a time when G. magnarostirs was very small in numbers, G. fortis INCREASED its beak size in ONE generation; that is, since there were no G. magnarostris to compete with, the G. fortis had all the T. cistoides to themselves. In the next generation beak size “popped-up” in size to the same degree that it decreased in size during the 2004 drought when their consumption of T. cistoides dropped drastically.

As a follow-up to this study, here is what the Grant’s propose: “Our findings should prove useful in designing realistic experiments, by identifying ecological context (high densities at the start of an environmental stress) and by estimating the magnitude of natural selection.

Here is what I consider to be another important point in all of this. From an ID perspective, this kind of an experiment is a complete waste of time. What would be valuable, OTOH, is an experiment wherein native Galapagos seeds are fed to controlled populations of G. magnarostris and G. fortis while observing changes to beak size (and other traits that are correlated). You see, ID really is “science”!

Finally, let’s remember that Kettlewell’s experiment and the Galapagos Finches are the Modern Synthesis’ great claims to fame. Well, I think they got it completely wrong. What do you think?

Comments
Hmmm, PaV, maybe you have all the research data on file that is required thru other massive amounts of research. What immediately came to mind was drug abuse symptoms. A female, impregnated who does not stop addiction to alcohol, crack, cocaine, even sugary diets. Is there any reason this is not the same type of observation but with a deleterious effect? Environment stimuli do matter, whether input thru chemical man made substances or natures protein growth hormones. There should be a lapse time and preceeding time frame for food or drugs to work thru systems. The "instant" evolution however works with current information still in the genome, like the blind fish, where 40% receive functional eyes from one hybridization. So, we see 1) informational overlap and "regain" by hybridization of species and 2) environment input factors that have instant change. None of this I see as related to macro-evolution or forces strong enough to create new information. It can only regain lost information or extend what is there in positive or negative forms.Michaels7
January 21, 2008
January
01
Jan
21
21
2008
05:25 PM
5
05
25
PM
PDT
Since I can't read the Grant pdf, I presume this is the article cited: Evolution of Character Displacement in Darwin's Finches; Peter R. Grant and B. Rosemary Grant Science 14 July 2006: Vol. 313. no. 5784, pp. 224 - 226 "Instant" Evolution Seen in Darwin's Finches, Study Says; Mason Inman for National Geographic News July 14, 2006 (printer formatted) Google Scholar lists the following citations to Grants' 2006 Science articleDLH
January 21, 2008
January
01
Jan
21
21
2008
05:06 PM
5
05
06
PM
PDT
Unletterd and Ordinary: Kinda like McDonalds in Japan. Exactly. Families coming over from Europe during the first half of last century experienced something like this too. Whether it's a protein that gets involved in the soma of an organism, and thus finds itself to the ova, or whether there is some way in which the protein is somehow translated into codable RNA that finds its way to the ova, something along those line is what I'm thinking of. There was an article about a year or so ago talking about non-Mendelian inheritance. ( Here's one link. ) I'm wondering if it's a more common mechanism that previously known.PaV
January 21, 2008
January
01
Jan
21
21
2008
04:54 PM
4
04
54
PM
PDT
SCheesman: The links are fixed now. Thanks for pointing that out. I put in the formatting from Windows, and, believe it or not, the quotation marks from Windows don't work.PaV
January 21, 2008
January
01
Jan
21
21
2008
04:46 PM
4
04
46
PM
PDT
Greetings! So your hypothesis is that the food releases some kind of growth element or trigger. Kinda like McDonalds in Japan. Little people becoming giants. Growth hormones in food that affect growth and development. Something along those lines. Sound experimental, sounds like, ah ah hmmm... oh science. I really would be interested in the results on such an experiment. Definitely worth an experiment or two or three or more.Unlettered and Ordinary
January 21, 2008
January
01
Jan
21
21
2008
03:58 PM
3
03
58
PM
PDT
Ditto for the National Geographic article.SCheesman
January 21, 2008
January
01
Jan
21
21
2008
03:36 PM
3
03
36
PM
PDT
PaV - the link to the paper doesn't seem to work.SCheesman
January 21, 2008
January
01
Jan
21
21
2008
03:35 PM
3
03
35
PM
PDT
So all the vaunted changes in beak sizes on the Galapagos Islands could be due to epigenetic factors, not natural selection at all? Down with Darwin! Bring back Lamarck! The mind reels. This is such a simple case of cause-effect, and I'd say publishable, at least as a letter. Well done, PaV!SCheesman
January 21, 2008
January
01
Jan
21
21
2008
03:28 PM
3
03
28
PM
PDT
LOL Gerry!PaV
January 21, 2008
January
01
Jan
21
21
2008
02:41 PM
2
02
41
PM
PDT
A significantly larger than normal beak - not present in the previous several generations - can be observed at Google Images with a search for "Jimmy Durante". Since no direct descendents with this particular characteristic have been found, it appears that Durantus Jimminus - a chance mutation of the pedestrian Durantus species - was rejected by natural selection and failed to reproduce for reasons as yet undiscovered. Further study is required, but it is clear that beak size is an important and defining characteristic of species.Gerry Rzeppa
January 21, 2008
January
01
Jan
21
21
2008
01:57 PM
1
01
57
PM
PDT
1 2 3

Leave a Reply